Monthly Archives: March 2015

Research Journal: Progress on Project

Progress as of Mar. 22

Upon looking at the peer reviews of my research proposal, I realized there were a few spots I needed to tighten up. I took care of that, and the results can be seen in my Google Drive folder. I still need to catch up on the annotated bibliography entries…

Progress as of Mar. 29

Thanks to class on Wednesday night, I was able to add more detail to my IRB protocol. Additionally, I received feedback about my survey questions that helped tailor the questions for more specific, more helpful results. I uploaded materials to IRBNet yesterday. However, when I tried fiddling around with Survey Monkey, it didn’t allow me to incorporate smileys into my answers. I tried Google Forms, and though that worked better, two different smileys (due to their components, I suppose) did not work with the survey. Lastly, I saw that the PDF of my protocol included “blank characters” in the place of the smileys.

So, a major overhaul was necessary. I requested an opening of my IRB package. I then changed my protocol and form to reflect changes to the study (going from Survey Monkey to Google Forms, mainly). I additionally added an image of the survey questions to the package, to make up for how the research protocol did not properly include the smileys. The package is locked again, and will hopefully be reviewed shortly.

Research Journal: Annotated Bibliographies 22-25

This post includes four different entries, as I have to make up the three that I missed and complete the one due on Sunday.

Due Sunday, Mar. 8
Delbaere, M., McQuarrie, E.F., & Phillips, B.J. (2011). Personification in Advertising: Using a Visual Metaphor to Trigger Anthropomorphism. Journal of Advertising, 40, 121-130.

Personification “taps into the deeply embedded human cognitive bias referred to as anthropomorphism — the tendency to attribute human qualities to things” (121). This article, which focuses on visual images in print advertising, posits that personification can encourage people to anthropomorphize things. Advertising sees positive results when subtle visual changes make static print ads appear to be engaged in some sort of human behavior. Personification is typically understood as a figure of speech that gives human qualities to inanimate objects. However, rhetorical personification invokes anthropomorphism as well as metaphorical processing. We’ve seen anthropomorphism in advertising with spokescharacters (e.g., the Geico gecko, M&M characters, etc.), but people now deem them as too obvious. Personification now tends to deviate from expectation in regard to style. For instance, an ad for Plus lotion depicts the bottle of lotion drinking a glass of water from a straw. There’s a puzzle to figure out; the personification is more complex. The authors of this article conducted a study in which 187 undergrads looked at ads for lotion from Plus, fruit-and-nut-bars from Mills, snack mix from Landers, and bleach from Excel. “Each participant saw only one version from any given ad set, but saw one ad for each of the four brands” (126). One ad featured personification, one did not, and the two others functioned as controls.
The hypotheses were as follows:

H1: Photorealistic pictures in an ad that show a product engaged in human behavior (i.e., a visual metaphor of personification) can trigger anthropomorphism in the absence of a verbal cue and without use of an animated character.
[…]
H2: Brands featured in ads that use personification will elicit (a) more attributions of brand personality, and (b) more emotional response than brands featured in ads that do not use personification.
[…]
H3: Brands featured in ads with personification will be liked more than brands featured in ads with no personification.
[…]
H4: The impact of personification metaphors on brand attitude, relative to nonpersonification metaphors, is mediated by the impact of personification on emotional response and brand personality attributions. (123-124)

In this study, ads with personification were seen as more effective than those that relied simply on (non-personified) metaphors.

Delbaere earned a Ph.D. in business; McQuarrie is a marketing professor; and Phillips teaches branding and advertising courses. Their experiences in business boosts their ethos in addressing how advertisements affect consumer activity.

On that note, I’ve noticed a clear trend between my research on anthropomorphism – for this class, for English 706, and for my thesis: Research on the effects of anthropomorphism and personification focuses heavily on advertising. Perhaps this i because ads are readily tangible for obvious reasons. However, the literature used in and concepts of this research are useful to my research on smileys, emojis, emoticons… the names often seem to be interchangeable.

Due Monday, Mar. 16
Lebduska, L. (2012). Emoji, Emoji, What for Art Thou? Harlot, 12.

The whole article appears on one page, hence the lack of page numbers in the reference above and in the in-text citations.

Lebduska looks at the history of the emoji, and argues that emojis are not a threat to traditional alphabetic literacy. Instead, they are a creative way to express ourselves; they even clarify tone or content in traditional alphabetic writing, sometimes. Emojis are culturally and contextually bound, and stretch linguistic conditions, as “writing […] always have a visual component mediated by a material world.” In 1982, Carnegie Mellon researchers gave birth to the emoticon – they used the now-iconic : ) (which WordPress automatically makes into 🙂 ) to indicate that the mentioned cutting of an elevator cable was simply a dose of dark humor. Emojis, originally created to boost teenage market share for mobile phone company DoCoMo, emerged almost 10 years later in Japan. They offered a wide array of activities, events, and objects, as well as more fleshed-out compositions than emoticons. They later became part of all web and mobile services in Japan. Google and Apple brought them to the Unicode Consortium in the mid-2000s; 722 codes for emoji were standardized in 2010. Emojis’ capabilities for more efficient communication have come under attack from some, but their meanings are more readily decipherable than, say, cuneiform or approaches from Sir Isaac Pitman and Gregg. However, only those with access to emojis can use them; it would be incorrect to claim universality. Additionally, they cannot act as universal because they represent white “as a universal, non-raced race.” Still, emojis offer people (with access) the ability to buffer confusion often seen in digital communication. They also offer the ability to obfuscate meaning, in regard to “unplain language,” sarcasm, and irony. There are arguments that emojis in themselves are not as heartfelt or genuine as textual messages. However, this regards textual messages as inherently heartfelt and genuine, as if someone cannot be shallow or unauthentic when composing through linguistic means. Lebduska concludes that emojis have just as much potential as words, and offer rich possibilities for the teaching of communication.

Lebduska teaches writing, and it’s refreshing to see an open attitude toward emojis, toward incorporating them into communication curriculum. As the approach taken in this article places specific importance on emoji-inclusive communication, it’s greatly inspired my project. While I knew that my work would be of importance – not only to this class and to my thesis, but also to communication in general – Lebduska’s scholarship connects resources from the past and present to set up a context for why this work is important.

Due Sunday, Mar. 22
Porter, J.E. (2009). Recovering Delivery for Visual Rhetoric. Computers and Composition, 26, 207-224.

Porter hopes “to resuscitate and remediate the rhetorical canon of delivery” (207). The perceived exclusivity of delivery to verbal communication contributed to its rarity in communication, English, and writing courses. He positions delivery as a techne, to give a broader picture of delivery, and proposes that digital delivery consists of the following five components:

  • Body and identity
  • Distribution and circulation
  • Access and accessibility
  • Interaction
  • Economics

Body and identity refer to online representations and performances of identity; they include gesture, voice, and dress (body), as well as sexual orientation, race, and class (identity). Our representations of ourselves online contribute to our ethos. Virtual spaces can recover visual and speaking bodies, and offer capabilities that duplicate those of the physical world. Distribution and circulation involve technological publishing options. Distribution refers to your packaging of a message to achieve a desired effect; circulation refers to how messages may be re-distributed without your direct intervention and have lives of their own. Access and accessibility involve audiences’ connectedness to Internet-based information. Access refers to how connected a particular group can be, while accessibility refers to how connected a particular group is. As it stands now, many in the general public do not have access to information distributed through digital means. We must try to reduce that gap. Interaction regards how digital designs allow and encourage people to engage with interfaces and with each other. While access is certainly important, engaging with people is crucial, too. Economics often involves legalities like copyright, fair use, and authorship. For instance, digital spaces offer capabilities that challenge industries built through nondigital means (e.g., the Napster brouhaha). Additionally, there’s plagiarism, which is easier now since the digital world makes sharing, and consequently stealing, rather easy. Porter concludes in saying that he hopes to develop rhetoric theory that is useful for the digital age.

Porter’s background in composition and rhetoric gives him major credibility when it comes to addressing contemporary rhetorical issues. As for my own research, emojis are perhaps most relevant to the body/identity component of digital delivery. In his discussion of bodies/identities, Porter discusses the simple : ) emoticon and avatars in Second Life. As Lebduska’s work pointed out, they are more complex than emoticons, but they are certainly not as complex as Second Life avatars – emojis won’t crash your computer, unless you did need to install that software update after all. This particular component of Porter’s work will be relevant to my report here – and likely my thesis, too!

Due Sunday, Mar. 29
Garrison, A., Remley, D., Thomas, P., & Wierszewski, E. (2011). Conventional Faces: Emoticons in Instant Messaging Discourse. Computers and Composition, 28, 112-125.

Garrison, et al. attempted to look at emoticons as their own conventions in IM (Instant Messaging) discourse. Typographic symbols contribute to the composition of emoticons, which have criticized in a broad number of fields. Mainstream preferences lean toward traditional spoken or written discourse, and adhere to the theory that language alone conveys meaning. Garrison, et al. use IM for personal and professional communication, and hope to discover conventions of emoticons in IM. “Due to the reliance on the speech/writing dichotomy,scholarship has been quick to label anything other than familiar forms of print-linguistic text as additive or ‘paralinguistic,’ thereby limiting the understanding of emoticons while not fully accounting for all their potential uses in IM discourse” (114). For instance, the first noted use of emoticons – again, a Carnegie Mellon professor in 1982 – was explicitly paralinguistic. However, people do not always or even necessarily use emoticons for paralinguistic purposes.

In this study, Garrison, et al. looked not at audience perception or the intent of the used language. Rather, they explored the features of language, hoping to look at “the new forms of language within the discourse” (115). They “analyzed an intact data set collected in 2005 by Christina Haas and Pamela Takayoshi for their study of language features of IM. This corpus of data included 59 transcripts of naturally occurring IM sessions, consisting of approximately 32,000 words produced by 108 interlocutors” (116).

Garrison, et al. coded by derivations based on the mouths of emoticons, as they are important in the user dynamic of American IM. I’ve included the following screenshot that refers to which emoticons they explored, as WordPress automatically changes emoticons to smileys/emojis:

garrison_screenshot

They additionally coded for placement – preceding, within, or following the textual component of the individual message. They also added context to the coding scheme.

Of the 59 IM transcripts explored, Garrison, et al. found 301 different uses of emoticons. The most used emoticons were (loosely translated) : ), : P, and ; ). Interestingly, in regard to placement, emoticons appeared at the end of a line alongside or in lieu of punctuation almost 50% of the time. Ultimately, Garrison, et al. found that emoticons are conventional and inventional, enhance punctuation; they recognize “that standards and conventions arise out of contextualized practices of CMC discourse” (124). Also, we have a more accurate interpretation of emoticons by understanding them as their own semiotic entities.

Garrison, et al. come from English departments. This bolsters their credibility when it comes to communication about, well, communication. The bit about emoticons acting as punctuation is particularly interesting, as I’ve seen at least one other article discussing images-as-punctuation. I wish I had come across this article before making my survey questions.

English 706: Annotated Bibliography 3

Scott, L.M. Images in Advertising: The Need for a Theory of Visual Rhetoric. The Journal of Consumer Research, 21, 252-273.

Scott aims to advocate for a theory of visual rhetoric in reorienting how people study advertising images. She argues against the assumption that pictures reflect reality, toward the idea that visuals make up a symbolic system based in social and cultural conventions. Scott conducts a literature review of existing scholarship on rhetorical theories as they pertain to advertisements, and looks at different advertisements in her discussion.

Advertising images, since they are rhetorical, must have certain capabilities: they must be capable of working toward through their invention, arrangement, and delivery. Scott argues that visual elements make up a symbolic system. An ad for a refillable lipstick from Coty showcases straightforward images of the product itself; textual information appears in bullet-point fashion. The images of the product are “relevant” to the advertising of the product; realistic photographs seemingly “copy” the product. Both contribute to the ad’s assumed objectivity. An ad for Clinique depicted the product emerged in a glass of ice soda water. This ad initially comes across as nonsensical and ineffective. However, people might understand that the product “is as refreshing as a tall glass of soda with a twist” if they were to restate the ad in verbal terms (254). This forces people to think abstractly, metaphorically about this ad. An ad for Max Factor, like the Coty ad, attempts to sell lipstick. This ad features two photos – one with rows of lipstick, the other with a closeup of a model’s lipstick-covered lips. However, the approach taken here significantly alters the perception of the product. The tiered rows of lipstick tubes resemble arrangements people see in the everyday, like a church choir; the closeup of the lips highlights the contrast between the skin and the lips, as well as the meticulousness of how the lipstick is applied. The ad relies on people to understand the context-sensitive manipulation of culturally-learned symbols. Ultimately, “the designs of the ads themselves anticipate a viewer who knows certain pictorial conventions and who shares visual experiences with the makers” (256).

Many paradigms regarding advertisements’ effects on consumer response can be tied back to copy theory, or the idea that pictures resemble reality. Copy theory refers to the idea that pictures resemble reality. When it comes to experimental studies, paradigms of how advertisements affect consumers’ responses can be grouped (admittedly broadly) into two categories: classical conditioning/affective response and information processing. In classical conditioning/affective response, images have a simple value (positive/negative, yes/no); images are iconic, pointing to happenings in the empirical world; and people do not invoke interpretive activity. This categorization suggests that people should react to images in the same way, which denies people’s various backgrounds, beliefs, and experiences. Information processing suggests that visuals can have a cognitive impact on people. In regard to large-scale descriptive studies of advertisements, paradigms include mechanical elements studies and content analysis. Mechanical elements studies examine the absence and presence of color, illustration, photography, and typography. Content analysis looks at the content in images; this disregards how images might be presented. Ultimately, Scott argues against the theory of copy theory, suggesting that it relies on the notion of a meditation-free perception of the visual. Additionally, seeing is very much a learned behavior.

In addressing invention, arrangement, and delivery, Scott points to several different ads at the end of her work. A lotion ad, in which different lotions “run” down a vertical structure, exemplifies invention; invention also paves the way for a Pandora’s box metaphor in a Djer-Kiss ad. In regard to arrangement, the typeface in a Honda ad changes the meanings of two uses of the word “this.” Delivery makes all the difference in the careful organization of sheets in an ad for Martex and a Wamsutta ad where all people see is the sheet.

My project for this course explores how a particular smiley works toward persuasion, even when its appearance might not suggest as much. While the smiley is not an advertisement, the concepts of visual rhetoric addressed by Scott will be relevant to my research. Additionally, Scott addresses the concept of the objective image, which we see in R. Belk (1977) and G. Kress and T. van Leeuwen (2006).

English 706: Asynchronous Assignment (Kress and Van Leeuwen)

  • List 3 connections between Kress and Van Leeuwen and 2-3 other readings we’ve covered. (Not every article you choose needs to have a connection to every other article. Look for common threads.)
  • List 3 thought provoking or provocative ideas. What makes them so?
  • List 3 questions you jotted down for yourself while reading. What sparked them for you and why?
  • Respond to 3 separate questions from your peer’s blogs by the following Tues. March 24. Don’t answer all 3 questions from 1 blog, spread the love.

Connecting with Readings
In their discussion of representation and interaction in visuals, G. Kress and T. van Leeuwen (2006) points out relations between involvement and the horizontal angle and power and the vertical angle (133-143). However, they seem to subscribe to the belief that visuals, like charts, diagrams, and maps, can be objective (143). This connects to R. Barthes’ (1977) concept of the denoted image. However, Barthes’ concept ultimately ends up being a social construct:

[T]he absence of code disintellectualizes the message because it seems to found in nature the signs of culture. This is without doubt an important historical paradox: the more technology develops the diffusion of information (and notably of images), the more it provides the means of masking the constructed meaning under the appearance of the given meaning. (159-160)

Kress and van Leeuwen also suggest that “directly frontal or perpendicular top-down angle” visuals are “objective” in presentation (143-144). However, they earlier point out that “[w]hen represented participants look at the viewer, vectors […] connect the participants with the viewer” and establish contact (117). This gaze sees the image acknowledging those who view it and constitutes an image act, which calls the viewer to some kind of action (117-118). This reminds me of b. hooks‘ (1995) discussion of the oppositional gaze. According to this work, staring carries power; “[e]ven in the worse [sic] circumstances of domination, the ability to manipulate one’s gaze in the face of structures of domination that would contain it, opens up the possibility of agency” (116).
Edit (Mar. 26, 2015): I just realized this was a reading from hooks not assigned for this course. I do address B.H. Welling in the questions, for what it’s worth

Thought-Provoking Ideas

  • When discussing the semiotic landscape, Kress and van Leeuwen ask if “the move from the verbal to the visual [is] a loss or a gain” (31). They suggest that there is no one correct answer to this question, but the idea of trading between loss and gain fascinates me.
  • When discussing materiality and meaning, Kress and van Leeuwen describe how the presentation of visuals are important in making meaning, not simply an empty vessel to display the visuals. This reminds me of Marshall McLuhan’s idea that the medium is the message; while such a proclamation might be a bit broad, it reminds me that we should not examine visuals (or anything else, for that matter) separately from their presentation.
  • Kress and van Leeuwen’s concept of the Given and the New (181, 183).
  • When discussing modality, Kress and van Leeuwen point out that a diagram isn’t necessarily “less real” than a photograph. After all, “[r]eality is in the eye of the beholder” (158).

Questions that Came to Mind; Why?

  • Kress and van Leeuwen seem to recognize the concept of the gaze (regardless of angle) but suggest that directly frontal visuals are “objective” in presentation. Considering that gazes can appear in directly frontal visuals, are Kress and van Leeuwen contradicting themselves somewhat? This particular question came to mind since the false construct of objectivity fascinates me.
  • Kress and van Leeuwen point out the concept of the Given-New relation in their discussion of the meaning of composition (181). In compositions with this kind of relation, the Given appears on the left, the New on the right; the Given represents what we know, what we accept, while the New represents what we perceive as problematic (181). For instance, Lorenzo Maitani’s The Creation of Eve fits this description, as the absolute God (left) creates the problematic Eve (right) (181). However, Michelangelo’s The Creation of Adam depicts Adam as the Given and God as the New, problematizing our concept of God (181, 183). What other visuals subvert expectations by switching up the Given and the New? I explore methods subversion in my project for this course and, ultimately, my thesis; the relation of the Given to the New has the power to problematize, question, and subvert our expectations.
  • While looking through Kress and van Leeuwen’s work, I came across their discussion of linear and non-linear compositions; I was reminded of B.H. Welling’s work on ecop*rnography. Would ecop*rnography be more likely to employ linear or non-linear compositions? Why? As strange as the particular terminology is, the word “ecop*rnography” captures how exploitative visual rhetoric can be when it comes to nature. I’m curious to see how others in the class feel in regard to which kind of composition is more exploitative.

References

  • Barthes, R. (1977). The Rhetoric of the Image. In Handa, C. (ed.), Visual Rhetoric in a Digital World: A Critical Sourcebook (152-163). New York: Bedford, St. Martin’s.
  • hooks, b. (1995). Black Vernacular: Architecture as Cultural Practice. In Handa, C. (ed.), Visual Rhetoric in a Digital World (395-400). New York: Bedford/St. Martin’s.
  • Kress, G., and van Leeuwen, T. (2006). Readings Images: The Grammar of Visual Design. New York: Routledge.
  • Welling, B.H. (2009). Ecop*rn: On the Limits of Visualizing the Nonhuman. In Dobrin, S.I., and Morey, S. (eds.), Ecosee: Image, Rhetoric, Nature. Albany: State University of New York.

Research Journal: Progress on Project (x2)

Looks like I forgot something due before spring break. While it might mean nothing for credit on the assignment, I might as well share

Progress as of Mar. 8

OK, while I missed this particular assignment, I would have had little to say at the time, as I was too busy worrying about IRB approval.

Progress as of Mar. 16

So, I made quite a bit of progress on the project. Honestly, I’m not so sure why I was worried about the whole IRB process. While I will be sending in the actual form for approval/exemption today (Mar. 16), the process was relatively easy and straightforward; the significant overlap of content in the research proposal and the research protocol helped make the process much easier.

That said, I settled on a topic for my research. (Finally!) I decided to ask how and why people use smileys and emojis, to explore the lack of scholarship on the subject. While research on queer representation would have been more interesting, this research can address overall use of smileys and emojis, which will tie into my thesis research, too – perhaps even more readily than research on queer representation.

You can access a PDF of everything (i.e., the research proposal, the IRB approval/exemption form, and the research protocol) here. I plan to begin collecting data next Monday.

Research Journal: Annotated Bibliographies 20-21

Cheek, J. (2010). Human Rights, Social Justice, and Qualitative Research: Questions and Hesitations about What We Say about What We Do. In Denzin, N.K., & Giardina, M.D. (eds), Qualitative Inquiry and Human Rights (100-111). Walnut Creek, CA: Left Coast Press.

Qualitative inquiry often regards human rights and social justice as important components of research. However, Cheek wonders if we think that our research alone works to advance human rights and social justice. What assumptions do we make about how our research works alongside human rights and social justice? Cheek explores hesitations she has about the connections between research and human rights and social justice. Her two points of hesitation are: Involvement and participation does not necessarily equal advancing social justice or human rights, and an alternate reading of a research program raises questions about what the doing actually does. Regarding the first point of hesitation, there is the assumption of a linear process that takes information from research to practice. Cheek addresses that we in qualitative research often hope to address some kind of sociological issue and use our research toward finding a solution. She then points out how we often come from an outside view: Who decided that researching a certain sociological topic was necessary? Who decided who would be involved in the research? In other words, are we restricting qualitative research to an ultimately conservative result, limiting progress of human rights and social justice in the process? Regarding the second point of hesitation, Cheek points out that the funding of specific research projects excludes other research projects from funding. After all, funding bodies tend to determine what and whom are researched, as well as how they are researched. Social justice and human rights are advanced, but this takes place within relatively conservative, certainly constructed parameters.

Cheek came from 10-plus years in a research program contributing to citizenship and social sustainability, in regard to care and services for older people. In other words, she has spent more than a decade in the field of research, which adds to her ethos. While my research will involve neither discussion with others nor funding, I am considering my research more carefully in regard to whose artifacts are included in the research, to why certain artifacts are privileged. In fact, I might need to change certain aspects of my proposal… again.

Erickson, F. (2010). Affirming Human Dignity in Qualitative Inquiry: Walking the Walk. In Denzin, N.K., & Giardina, M.D. (eds), Qualitative Inquiry and Human Rights (112-122). Walnut Creek, CA: Left Coast Press.

Erickson points out that, in the best circumstances, “qualitative social research advances human rights and affirms human dignity by seeking and telling the truth about what particular people do in their everyday lives and about what their actions mean to them” (113). As such, we understand the stereotyping of subalterns (i.e., black slaves being happy and content in their oppression, commercializing people native to Hawaii for tourist purposes, etc.) as violations of human rights. Qualitative research, ideally, treats subjects with honor and respect, but much inquiry of a qualitative nature ultimately amounted to Foucauldian panoptical surveillance. After all, qualitative researchers conventionally boast more power than the qualitatively researched; such privilege can (and often does) prevent researchers from seeing limits and important differences. There’s also the issue of pursuing “a definite truth,” when we have come to understand such a thing as contextualized truth(s).

Erickson goes on to review previous and contemporary methods of qualitative research, highlighting worthy goals and the difficulties in achieving them. These methods include: ethnography, critical ethnography, autoethnography, participatory action research (or collaborative action research), and arts-based qualitative research and performance ethnography. Ethnography simply refers to accounts of how people go about their everyday lives; this research usually focuses on some kind of subaltern. While the intentions are noble (usually), those who exist outside of the studied subaltern group often find difficulty in relating to the group of people they are studying. Critical ethnography examines oppression, including that of a subaltern group against itself. While this sort of inquiry can teach us, how do we communicate with oppressors within a subaltern group honestly about our research? Autoethnography sees someone within a group conducting research on that group. This inquiry avoids more readily than ethnography and critical ethnography. However, researchers might fail to see what’s right in front of them, and they might be blind to their own privilege, too. Participatory action research involves the researchers and the researched collaborating and working toward a change for the better, rather than enacting a traditional researcher-researched relationship. Arts-based qualitative research and performance ethnography see researchers shunning the notion of objectivity in their work, and conveying their research in more vivid, literary ways. Still, the researchers must check their bias(es), and not every researcher can convey their work and findings through more personal styles of writing.

Erickson concludes that we must question the good our research can do. We should continue to conduct qualitative research, but we should keep ourselves in check more than qualitative researchers of previous generations did.

Erickson’s research interests include the study of social interaction as a learning environment, the anthropology of education, and (obviously) ethnographic research methods. His interests make him an appropriate researcher for this topic. As for how this will function in my own research, I must again reiterate that I will not converse with others for my research in this course. In the end, my (thesis) research will work toward observing online activity – queer subversion, more specifically – though this specific project will focus on finding relevant artifacts. Still, it is important that I do not contribute to the panoptical surveillance perpetuated by former qualitative studies, here or in my broader research. Rather, I should resist any temptations to sensationalize or marginalize, and additionally raise questions regarding if such temptations arise in the first place. I believe that my being a part of the subaltern group focused on in my research will help me in this regard.

Research Journal: Progress on Project

Between the first and second proposal, I changed not only my angle for this research project but also the way in which I would gather results. I had wanted to conduct surveys, initially, but the time constraints were an issue. Fortunately, the angle I’m now taking does not necessitate surveys. However, since I’m focusing on why certain literature hardly exists, I will need to broaden my horizons to look at overall concepts of art. I plan to look at how then-new forms of art are always marginalized and considered as “lesser than” others. For instance, when film became popular, it was seen as greatl inferior to the theatre. Likewise, television became more “movie-like” and was taken more seriously when video websites like YouTube entered the pucblic consciousness. As for why little literature exists in regard to gay but not sexual artifacts… I’m still trying to figure that one out, but it seems like an issue of constructing identity.