Tag Archives: Research Journal

Research Journal: Project Progress (Extra #6)

Dr. Rodrigo,

As I put the finishing touches on my research report, I took into account what you said about discussing limitations, that we should explain how we might solve our problems the next time around instead of simply listing them. I added a little bit to the conclusions section of my report; I’d discussed possible solutions with some of my limitations but not all of them.

Additionally, I should have considered this for my presentation. Simply saying what went wrong likely gives little confidence that I’ll do well with similar endeavors in the future.

Research Journal: Project Progress (Extra #5 – I’m Free… Maybe)

So, I’ve just completed a draft of the research project.

I used the definitions provided by participants to code data as either direct, indirect, or nonexistent – in relation to Barthes’ threefold image. Direct implies that participants acknowledged the literal structure of smileys and explained why people use them; indirect implies that participants explained why people use smileys but said nothing of their literal structures; and nonexistent implies that participants described only the denoted image and failed to explain why people may use smileys.

Research Journal: Project Progress (Extra #4)

I just realized something within the last few days…

When I began my research, I explored my survey results for responses that indicated participants’ clear-cut recognition of anthropomorphic form. In other words, I hoped their definitions of smileys would include their (admittedly crude) resemblance to human faces.

However, upon further reflection, I may have held too strict a notion of anthropomorphism when I incorporated that concept into my study. Anthropomorphic form refers to the visual nature of something, but anthropomorphism refers to the general, often subconscious process of attributing the qualities of people to living beings that are not people (including animals) and inanimate objects.

Some participants referred to emotions in their definitions of smileys. While this doesn’t necessarily refer to anthropomorphic form, it does refer to anthropomorphism in general.

My next, likely last research journal update will likely address survey results, etc.

Research Journal: Annotated Bibliographies 26-27

Due Apr. 5, 2015

Bartholomew, M.K., Schoppe-Sullivan, S.J., Glassman, M., & Kamp Dush, C.M. (2012). New Parents’ Facebook Use at the Transition to Parenthood. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 61, 455-469.

Bartholomew, et al. examine how new parents use Facebook through the concept of social capital, which centers on how people accrue resources through their relationships with other people. “[C]ontextual sources of support, includ-ing the parents’ social networks (e.g., friends, neighbors, relatives) [have been regarded] as important influences on parental adjustment” (455). Bartholomew, et al. suggest that Facebook’s status as a popular online social network may allow new parents to gain social capital.

In their study, they asked four questions: “How do new parents use Facebook? How does new parents’ Facebook use change over the transition to parenthood? Do new mothers and fathers differ in their Facebook use at the transition to parenthood? How is new parents’ Facebook use associated with their adjustment to parenthood?” (457). Bartholomew, et al. took their data “from the fourth and final phase of a short-term longitudinal study of new fathers’ and mothers’ adjustment to parenthood (the New Parents Project)” (458). Expectant parents participating in the study were required to be at least 18 years old, able to read/speak English, cohabiting or married, the biological parents of the expected child, and expecting their first child. Couples signed on for the study during the third trimester of pregnancy. The study ultimately explored 182 couples.

Some fascinating results form the study are as follows:

  • “New mothers who reported having Face-book accounts were significantly younger thannew mothers who reported not having Facebook accounts” (461).
  • Mothers reported greater satisfaction with parenting when they also reported “that a greater proportion of their Facebook friends were family members or relatives” (463).
  • Mothers reported greater satisfaction with parenting when they also reported that friends were likelier to comment on photos of their children.
  • Mothers reported higher levels of parenting-related stress when they frequently visited and managed their Facebook accounts.

Bartholomew and Glassman come from concentrations in human development and family sciences; Schoppe-Sullivan teaches human sciences and psychology; and Kamp Dush teaches human sciences and sociology. The blend of their similar educational backgrounds makes for an interesting study – and boosts their credibility when it comes to this particular study, too. However, for my particular research, I was hoping this article would focus more on Facebook literacy. Thankfully, my next annotated bibliography focused on that concept…

Due Apr. 12, 2015

Bowen, L.M. (2011). Resisting Age Bias in Digital Literacy Research. College Composition and Communication, 62, 586-607.

Bowen “argue[s] that literacy researchers should pay greater attention to elder writers, readers, and learners” (586). She visited an 81-year-old friend and checked her email at the house. Her friend’s house computer was not turned on; this spurred thought about a predominant ideology concerning literacy narratives, which “privileges the literacies and literate activities of younger people and figures elder adults as digitally deficient” (587). Literacies tied to Web 2.0 are viewed as being tied to younger people; Web 1.0-centric literacies are often seen as being for the elderly.

MORE…

JSTOR link for future reference: http://www.jstor.org.proxy.lib.odu.edu/stable/pdf/23006907.pdf?acceptTC=true

Research Journal: Project Progress (Extra # 3)

After today, I have just three days to collect more results. At this time (about 6pm ET on Sunday, Apr. 12), I have 18 different responses to my survey. That’s hardly a poor number of responses, but I was hoping for more. I was also hoping for more responses that proved my thesis, though none of the responses disprove my thesis.

Assuming that I conduct a similar yet more specific survey for my thesis, the responses will be helpful in shaping my questions toward being more specific about what I’m looking for.

I must also work on my annotated bibliographies, which I hope to focus on technology limitations.

Research Journal: Project Progress (Extra #2)

After posting the same status update about the survey over and over again, I realize that I might not receive any additional survey responses – the last response was turned in two days ago. At the moment, I have 16 different responses.

Though I will more thoroughly explore my results next week, I’ve found some data that backs up my (hypo)thesis and none that directly contradicts it. As I mentioned on Wednesday, the terminology was less clear than it should have been, which likely affected how people answered their questions. (Translation: Framing is everything. Marshall McLuhan would yell, “I told you so!”)

I’ve realized yet another limitation to my study. I should have considered posting survey links/invitations on Twitter on a daily basis, too. I have more Twitter followers than I do Facebook friends. As expected, the folks I regularly converse with on Twitter are more diverse when it comes to their overall backgrounds than my Facebook friends.

Oh, and I also need to write two annotated bibliographies (one of which was due this past Sunday).

Research Journal: Project Progress

This is the journal entry that was due on Sunday, now arriving on Wednesday.

Anyway…

I began to collect results on Monday. Google Forms is supporting the whole survey, thus contributing to Google’s eventual takeover of the world alongside Oprah and Disney, although I can’t say that I’d be complaining about that particular trio being in charge. But I heavily digress…

My collecting of data is more or less a done deal – people take the survey, Google collects the results, and I explore them next week after I stop collecting results. However, the analyzing of this data could be a bit of a challenge…

I had already seen some limitations with my study before I began to collect results. But I see even more limitations and issues with my survey upon looking at some of the survey responses. Some aspects of my survey that could have been more robust include:

  • Terminology – I used the word “smiley” when I was technically referring to “emoji.” While they are similar in composition, emojis are part of the Unicode Consortium, while smileys are not.
  • Descriptions – When I asked the question about people linking to Facebook posts, some have expressed that they do not know how to link to their posts.
  • Questions themselves – I could have asked the question about Facebook more broadly, as some say that they used smileys in comments and Facebook Messenger – but not in actual posts/status updates. Additionally, I could have asked questions about how people have used smileys in recent text messages, emails, etc.

Research Journal: Project Progress (Extra #1)

First things first, IRB approved my research for exemption! However, the IRB site said that requests should be in rather early, so I had anticipated a somewhat lengthy IRB process. For that reason, I plan(ned) to begin collecting data on Apr. 6. So, I must wait several days before seeking data.

Looking at the schedule, I’ve begun to draft my report. At the time of this post (around 2pm on Wednesday, Apr. 1), I need to add sources to my literature review. I plan/hope to use all four annotated bibliographies that I turned in on Sunday in my research report, as all directly tie into my research. I also need to make sure that my literature review functions like one – I can summarize and compose annotated bibliographies, but throwing everything into a cohesive literature review has been a problem when it comes to this course – and my thesis, too.

After checking out the readings for last week tonight, I saw on the schedule where we’re supposed to connect theory to our research. I wasn’t sure that my particular project incorporated theory into its framework – and I’m still a bit wary that it is. However, I suppose that my research could (very loosely) be considered feminist. Feminist research tends to favor subjectivity and the gathering of robust perspectives from those who participate in the research. The final question on my survey prompts participants to discuss several Facebook posts and why they incorporated smileys into them. This gives participants the opportunity to shape their own narratives.

Research Journal: Progress on Project

Progress as of Mar. 22

Upon looking at the peer reviews of my research proposal, I realized there were a few spots I needed to tighten up. I took care of that, and the results can be seen in my Google Drive folder. I still need to catch up on the annotated bibliography entries…

Progress as of Mar. 29

Thanks to class on Wednesday night, I was able to add more detail to my IRB protocol. Additionally, I received feedback about my survey questions that helped tailor the questions for more specific, more helpful results. I uploaded materials to IRBNet yesterday. However, when I tried fiddling around with Survey Monkey, it didn’t allow me to incorporate smileys into my answers. I tried Google Forms, and though that worked better, two different smileys (due to their components, I suppose) did not work with the survey. Lastly, I saw that the PDF of my protocol included “blank characters” in the place of the smileys.

So, a major overhaul was necessary. I requested an opening of my IRB package. I then changed my protocol and form to reflect changes to the study (going from Survey Monkey to Google Forms, mainly). I additionally added an image of the survey questions to the package, to make up for how the research protocol did not properly include the smileys. The package is locked again, and will hopefully be reviewed shortly.

Research Journal: Annotated Bibliographies 22-25

This post includes four different entries, as I have to make up the three that I missed and complete the one due on Sunday.

Due Sunday, Mar. 8
Delbaere, M., McQuarrie, E.F., & Phillips, B.J. (2011). Personification in Advertising: Using a Visual Metaphor to Trigger Anthropomorphism. Journal of Advertising, 40, 121-130.

Personification “taps into the deeply embedded human cognitive bias referred to as anthropomorphism — the tendency to attribute human qualities to things” (121). This article, which focuses on visual images in print advertising, posits that personification can encourage people to anthropomorphize things. Advertising sees positive results when subtle visual changes make static print ads appear to be engaged in some sort of human behavior. Personification is typically understood as a figure of speech that gives human qualities to inanimate objects. However, rhetorical personification invokes anthropomorphism as well as metaphorical processing. We’ve seen anthropomorphism in advertising with spokescharacters (e.g., the Geico gecko, M&M characters, etc.), but people now deem them as too obvious. Personification now tends to deviate from expectation in regard to style. For instance, an ad for Plus lotion depicts the bottle of lotion drinking a glass of water from a straw. There’s a puzzle to figure out; the personification is more complex. The authors of this article conducted a study in which 187 undergrads looked at ads for lotion from Plus, fruit-and-nut-bars from Mills, snack mix from Landers, and bleach from Excel. “Each participant saw only one version from any given ad set, but saw one ad for each of the four brands” (126). One ad featured personification, one did not, and the two others functioned as controls.
The hypotheses were as follows:

H1: Photorealistic pictures in an ad that show a product engaged in human behavior (i.e., a visual metaphor of personification) can trigger anthropomorphism in the absence of a verbal cue and without use of an animated character.
[…]
H2: Brands featured in ads that use personification will elicit (a) more attributions of brand personality, and (b) more emotional response than brands featured in ads that do not use personification.
[…]
H3: Brands featured in ads with personification will be liked more than brands featured in ads with no personification.
[…]
H4: The impact of personification metaphors on brand attitude, relative to nonpersonification metaphors, is mediated by the impact of personification on emotional response and brand personality attributions. (123-124)

In this study, ads with personification were seen as more effective than those that relied simply on (non-personified) metaphors.

Delbaere earned a Ph.D. in business; McQuarrie is a marketing professor; and Phillips teaches branding and advertising courses. Their experiences in business boosts their ethos in addressing how advertisements affect consumer activity.

On that note, I’ve noticed a clear trend between my research on anthropomorphism – for this class, for English 706, and for my thesis: Research on the effects of anthropomorphism and personification focuses heavily on advertising. Perhaps this i because ads are readily tangible for obvious reasons. However, the literature used in and concepts of this research are useful to my research on smileys, emojis, emoticons… the names often seem to be interchangeable.

Due Monday, Mar. 16
Lebduska, L. (2012). Emoji, Emoji, What for Art Thou? Harlot, 12.

The whole article appears on one page, hence the lack of page numbers in the reference above and in the in-text citations.

Lebduska looks at the history of the emoji, and argues that emojis are not a threat to traditional alphabetic literacy. Instead, they are a creative way to express ourselves; they even clarify tone or content in traditional alphabetic writing, sometimes. Emojis are culturally and contextually bound, and stretch linguistic conditions, as “writing […] always have a visual component mediated by a material world.” In 1982, Carnegie Mellon researchers gave birth to the emoticon – they used the now-iconic : ) (which WordPress automatically makes into 🙂 ) to indicate that the mentioned cutting of an elevator cable was simply a dose of dark humor. Emojis, originally created to boost teenage market share for mobile phone company DoCoMo, emerged almost 10 years later in Japan. They offered a wide array of activities, events, and objects, as well as more fleshed-out compositions than emoticons. They later became part of all web and mobile services in Japan. Google and Apple brought them to the Unicode Consortium in the mid-2000s; 722 codes for emoji were standardized in 2010. Emojis’ capabilities for more efficient communication have come under attack from some, but their meanings are more readily decipherable than, say, cuneiform or approaches from Sir Isaac Pitman and Gregg. However, only those with access to emojis can use them; it would be incorrect to claim universality. Additionally, they cannot act as universal because they represent white “as a universal, non-raced race.” Still, emojis offer people (with access) the ability to buffer confusion often seen in digital communication. They also offer the ability to obfuscate meaning, in regard to “unplain language,” sarcasm, and irony. There are arguments that emojis in themselves are not as heartfelt or genuine as textual messages. However, this regards textual messages as inherently heartfelt and genuine, as if someone cannot be shallow or unauthentic when composing through linguistic means. Lebduska concludes that emojis have just as much potential as words, and offer rich possibilities for the teaching of communication.

Lebduska teaches writing, and it’s refreshing to see an open attitude toward emojis, toward incorporating them into communication curriculum. As the approach taken in this article places specific importance on emoji-inclusive communication, it’s greatly inspired my project. While I knew that my work would be of importance – not only to this class and to my thesis, but also to communication in general – Lebduska’s scholarship connects resources from the past and present to set up a context for why this work is important.

Due Sunday, Mar. 22
Porter, J.E. (2009). Recovering Delivery for Visual Rhetoric. Computers and Composition, 26, 207-224.

Porter hopes “to resuscitate and remediate the rhetorical canon of delivery” (207). The perceived exclusivity of delivery to verbal communication contributed to its rarity in communication, English, and writing courses. He positions delivery as a techne, to give a broader picture of delivery, and proposes that digital delivery consists of the following five components:

  • Body and identity
  • Distribution and circulation
  • Access and accessibility
  • Interaction
  • Economics

Body and identity refer to online representations and performances of identity; they include gesture, voice, and dress (body), as well as sexual orientation, race, and class (identity). Our representations of ourselves online contribute to our ethos. Virtual spaces can recover visual and speaking bodies, and offer capabilities that duplicate those of the physical world. Distribution and circulation involve technological publishing options. Distribution refers to your packaging of a message to achieve a desired effect; circulation refers to how messages may be re-distributed without your direct intervention and have lives of their own. Access and accessibility involve audiences’ connectedness to Internet-based information. Access refers to how connected a particular group can be, while accessibility refers to how connected a particular group is. As it stands now, many in the general public do not have access to information distributed through digital means. We must try to reduce that gap. Interaction regards how digital designs allow and encourage people to engage with interfaces and with each other. While access is certainly important, engaging with people is crucial, too. Economics often involves legalities like copyright, fair use, and authorship. For instance, digital spaces offer capabilities that challenge industries built through nondigital means (e.g., the Napster brouhaha). Additionally, there’s plagiarism, which is easier now since the digital world makes sharing, and consequently stealing, rather easy. Porter concludes in saying that he hopes to develop rhetoric theory that is useful for the digital age.

Porter’s background in composition and rhetoric gives him major credibility when it comes to addressing contemporary rhetorical issues. As for my own research, emojis are perhaps most relevant to the body/identity component of digital delivery. In his discussion of bodies/identities, Porter discusses the simple : ) emoticon and avatars in Second Life. As Lebduska’s work pointed out, they are more complex than emoticons, but they are certainly not as complex as Second Life avatars – emojis won’t crash your computer, unless you did need to install that software update after all. This particular component of Porter’s work will be relevant to my report here – and likely my thesis, too!

Due Sunday, Mar. 29
Garrison, A., Remley, D., Thomas, P., & Wierszewski, E. (2011). Conventional Faces: Emoticons in Instant Messaging Discourse. Computers and Composition, 28, 112-125.

Garrison, et al. attempted to look at emoticons as their own conventions in IM (Instant Messaging) discourse. Typographic symbols contribute to the composition of emoticons, which have criticized in a broad number of fields. Mainstream preferences lean toward traditional spoken or written discourse, and adhere to the theory that language alone conveys meaning. Garrison, et al. use IM for personal and professional communication, and hope to discover conventions of emoticons in IM. “Due to the reliance on the speech/writing dichotomy,scholarship has been quick to label anything other than familiar forms of print-linguistic text as additive or ‘paralinguistic,’ thereby limiting the understanding of emoticons while not fully accounting for all their potential uses in IM discourse” (114). For instance, the first noted use of emoticons – again, a Carnegie Mellon professor in 1982 – was explicitly paralinguistic. However, people do not always or even necessarily use emoticons for paralinguistic purposes.

In this study, Garrison, et al. looked not at audience perception or the intent of the used language. Rather, they explored the features of language, hoping to look at “the new forms of language within the discourse” (115). They “analyzed an intact data set collected in 2005 by Christina Haas and Pamela Takayoshi for their study of language features of IM. This corpus of data included 59 transcripts of naturally occurring IM sessions, consisting of approximately 32,000 words produced by 108 interlocutors” (116).

Garrison, et al. coded by derivations based on the mouths of emoticons, as they are important in the user dynamic of American IM. I’ve included the following screenshot that refers to which emoticons they explored, as WordPress automatically changes emoticons to smileys/emojis:

garrison_screenshot

They additionally coded for placement – preceding, within, or following the textual component of the individual message. They also added context to the coding scheme.

Of the 59 IM transcripts explored, Garrison, et al. found 301 different uses of emoticons. The most used emoticons were (loosely translated) : ), : P, and ; ). Interestingly, in regard to placement, emoticons appeared at the end of a line alongside or in lieu of punctuation almost 50% of the time. Ultimately, Garrison, et al. found that emoticons are conventional and inventional, enhance punctuation; they recognize “that standards and conventions arise out of contextualized practices of CMC discourse” (124). Also, we have a more accurate interpretation of emoticons by understanding them as their own semiotic entities.

Garrison, et al. come from English departments. This bolsters their credibility when it comes to communication about, well, communication. The bit about emoticons acting as punctuation is particularly interesting, as I’ve seen at least one other article discussing images-as-punctuation. I wish I had come across this article before making my survey questions.